Bouletic bias in German questions: Evidence from production and perception

Lade...
Vorschaubild
Dateien
Kutscheid-2-c5lxtqbie55a5_flat.pdf
Kutscheid-2-c5lxtqbie55a5_flat.pdfGröße: 8.04 MBDownloads: 63
Datum
2024
Herausgeber:innen
Kontakt
ISSN der Zeitschrift
Electronic ISSN
ISBN
Bibliografische Daten
Verlag
Schriftenreihe
Auflagebezeichnung
DOI (zitierfähiger Link)
ArXiv-ID
Internationale Patentnummer
Angaben zur Forschungsförderung
Projekt
Open Access-Veröffentlichung
Open Access Green
Sammlungen
Core Facility der Universität Konstanz
Phonlab
Gesperrt bis
Titel in einer weiteren Sprache
Publikationstyp
Dissertation
Publikationsstatus
Published
Erschienen in
Zusammenfassung

This thesis examines the syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic characteristics of questions with a bouletic bias and how they are prosodically realized by German speakers. Biased questions are not neutral towards the answer from the addressee but carry implicit information about the questioner's predispositions or attitudes towards the possible answers (e.g., Büring & Gunlogson, 2000; Krifka, 2011; Reese, 2007; Sudo, 2013; van Rooy & Safarova, amongst others). In questions with a bouletic bias this attitude is a desire towards the proposition (p) of the question or its negation (¬p). The theoretical part of this dissertation establishes that biased questions can be realized with the same syntactic surface structure as their neutral counterparts (e.g., Pheby, 1975; van Rooy & Šafárová, 2003) and that they are questions both in the semantic and pragmatic sense (e.g., Krifka, 2011; Reese, 2007). While bias can be marked through morpho-syntactic structures like certain question types that are inherently biased (e.g., negative polar questions, tag-questions) or negative polarity items (e.g., to bat an eye), we argue that the pragmatic function of a biased question is detached from its grammatical form and that prosody alone is sufficient to add the speaker bias on a pragmatic level. As such, biased questions provide a case study of the interaction of prosody and pragmatics (cf. Reese, 2007: ix). In a series of experimental investigations, we analysed the production, perception, interpretation, and utilization of biased questions, integrating both quantitative and qualitative data. The conducted production studies yielded results in two ways. Firstly, it became evident that alternative questions (e.g., Kommst du mit oder bleibst du zu Hause? ‘Are you coming along, or are you staying at home?’) appear to be more suitable to pose an unbiased, neutral question, while polar questions (e.g., Kommst du mit? Are you coming along?’) are typically used to formulate a biased question (confirming assumptions by Biezma & Rawlins, 2012); and secondly that in string-identical questions speakers use prosodic marking strategies to indirectly express their underlying intention or desire for one of the possible answers. To this end they employ categorical and gradual differences in the intonation contour, i.e., the produced tonal height throughout the utterance, as well as in the speech rate. Specifically, higher pitch and a larger pitch range in the final rise is associated with neutral questions while lower pitch, a shallower utterance final rise and a slower speech rate are associated with bouletic bias. Statements obtained in a qualitative interrogation showed that speakers intentionally devote these prosodic aspects to the marking of their desires. This was indirectly corroborated by the results of a decision task: Listeners are capable to distinguish between biased and neutral questions when hearing them in isolation, i.e., without any further contextual information, suggesting that prosody alone is a sufficient cue to speaker bias. Participants furthermore demonstrated that they are aware of the purpose and benefits of questions with a bouletic bias in natural communication and that they assign specific meanings (correlated with the connotative meaning and intended discourse function of biased questions) to the intonation contours that were identified as prototypical for the different pragmatic functions. Although the exact interpretive contribution of any prosodic cue is highly context dependent and we therefore restrain from postulating a one-to-one relation between prosodic form and pragmatic feature, the results still highlight the importance of prosody in conveying certain discourse functions. Overall, this work contributes to our understanding of how bias is expressed and perceived in spoken communication and on the role of prosody to signal pragmatic functions. Thereby, the empirical findings allow for important theoretical conclusions at the prosody-pragmatics interface. We argue that bouletic bias might be modeled as a multilayered pragmatic function consisting of a complex speech-act type (following Reese, 2007) in which they are simultaneously questions and requests, as well as an additional propositional attitude (towards p or ¬p), both being marked by a bundle of distinct prosodic cues.

Zusammenfassung in einer weiteren Sprache
Fachgebiet (DDC)
400 Sprachwissenschaft, Linguistik
Schlagwörter
Prosody-pragmatics interface, biased questions, German, Prosody, Production, Perception
Konferenz
Rezension
undefined / . - undefined, undefined
Forschungsvorhaben
Organisationseinheiten
Zeitschriftenheft
Datensätze
Zitieren
ISO 690KUTSCHEID, Sophie, 2024. Bouletic bias in German questions: Evidence from production and perception [Dissertation]. Konstanz: Universität Konstanz
BibTex
@phdthesis{Kutscheid2024Boule-70572,
  year={2024},
  title={Bouletic bias in German questions: Evidence from production and perception},
  author={Kutscheid, Sophie},
  address={Konstanz},
  school={Universität Konstanz}
}
RDF
<rdf:RDF
    xmlns:dcterms="http://purl.org/dc/terms/"
    xmlns:dc="http://purl.org/dc/elements/1.1/"
    xmlns:rdf="http://www.w3.org/1999/02/22-rdf-syntax-ns#"
    xmlns:bibo="http://purl.org/ontology/bibo/"
    xmlns:dspace="http://digital-repositories.org/ontologies/dspace/0.1.0#"
    xmlns:foaf="http://xmlns.com/foaf/0.1/"
    xmlns:void="http://rdfs.org/ns/void#"
    xmlns:xsd="http://www.w3.org/2001/XMLSchema#" > 
  <rdf:Description rdf:about="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/server/rdf/resource/123456789/70572">
    <dspace:isPartOfCollection rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/server/rdf/resource/123456789/45"/>
    <dc:contributor>Kutscheid, Sophie</dc:contributor>
    <dc:date rdf:datatype="http://www.w3.org/2001/XMLSchema#dateTime">2024-08-12T11:46:59Z</dc:date>
    <dcterms:isPartOf rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/server/rdf/resource/123456789/45"/>
    <dcterms:available rdf:datatype="http://www.w3.org/2001/XMLSchema#dateTime">2024-08-12T11:46:59Z</dcterms:available>
    <dcterms:hasPart rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/bitstream/123456789/70572/4/Kutscheid-2-c5lxtqbie55a5_flat.pdf"/>
    <dcterms:issued>2024</dcterms:issued>
    <foaf:homepage rdf:resource="http://localhost:8080/"/>
    <dc:language>eng</dc:language>
    <dcterms:abstract>This thesis examines the syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic characteristics of questions with a bouletic bias and how they are prosodically realized by German speakers. Biased questions are not neutral towards the answer from the addressee but carry implicit information about the questioner's predispositions or attitudes towards the possible answers (e.g., Büring &amp; Gunlogson, 2000; Krifka, 2011; Reese, 2007; Sudo, 2013; van Rooy &amp; Safarova, amongst others). In questions with a bouletic bias this attitude is a desire towards the proposition (p) of the question or its negation (¬p). The theoretical part of this dissertation establishes that biased questions can be realized with the same syntactic surface structure as their neutral counterparts (e.g., Pheby, 1975; van Rooy &amp; Šafárová, 2003) and that they are questions both in the semantic and pragmatic sense (e.g., Krifka, 2011; Reese, 2007). While bias can be marked through morpho-syntactic structures like certain question types that are inherently biased (e.g., negative polar questions, tag-questions) or negative polarity items (e.g., to bat an eye), we argue that the pragmatic function of a biased question is detached from its grammatical form and that prosody alone is sufficient to add the speaker bias on a pragmatic level. As such, biased questions provide a case study of the interaction of prosody and pragmatics (cf. Reese, 2007: ix).
In a series of experimental investigations, we analysed the production, perception, interpretation, and utilization of biased questions, integrating both quantitative and qualitative data. The conducted production studies yielded results in two ways. Firstly, it became evident that alternative questions (e.g., Kommst du mit oder bleibst du zu Hause? ‘Are you coming along, or are you staying at home?’) appear to be more suitable to pose an unbiased, neutral question, while polar questions (e.g., Kommst du mit? Are you coming along?’) are typically used to formulate a biased question (confirming assumptions by Biezma &amp; Rawlins, 2012); and secondly that in string-identical questions speakers use prosodic marking strategies to indirectly express their underlying intention or desire for one of the possible answers. To this end they employ categorical and gradual differences in the intonation contour, i.e., the produced tonal height throughout the utterance, as well as in the speech rate. Specifically, higher pitch and a larger pitch range in the final rise is associated with neutral questions while lower pitch, a shallower utterance final rise and a slower speech rate are associated with bouletic bias. Statements obtained in a qualitative interrogation showed that speakers intentionally devote these prosodic aspects to the marking of their desires. This was indirectly corroborated by the results of a decision task: Listeners are capable to distinguish between biased and neutral questions when hearing them in isolation, i.e., without any further contextual information, suggesting that prosody alone is a sufficient cue to speaker bias. Participants furthermore demonstrated that they are aware of the purpose and benefits of questions with a bouletic bias in natural communication and that they assign specific meanings (correlated with the connotative meaning and intended discourse function of biased questions) to the intonation contours that were identified as prototypical for the different pragmatic functions. Although the exact interpretive contribution of any prosodic cue is highly context dependent and we therefore restrain from postulating a one-to-one relation between prosodic form and pragmatic feature, the results still highlight the importance of prosody in conveying certain discourse functions.
Overall, this work contributes to our understanding of how bias is expressed and perceived in spoken communication and on the role of prosody to signal pragmatic functions. Thereby, the empirical findings allow for important theoretical conclusions at the prosody-pragmatics interface. We argue that bouletic bias might be modeled as a multilayered pragmatic function consisting of a complex speech-act type (following Reese, 2007) in which they are simultaneously questions and requests, as well as an additional propositional attitude (towards p or ¬p), both being marked by a bundle of distinct prosodic cues.</dcterms:abstract>
    <dcterms:rights rdf:resource="https://rightsstatements.org/page/InC/1.0/"/>
    <dc:rights>terms-of-use</dc:rights>
    <dcterms:title>Bouletic bias in German questions: Evidence from production and perception</dcterms:title>
    <void:sparqlEndpoint rdf:resource="http://localhost/fuseki/dspace/sparql"/>
    <dspace:hasBitstream rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/bitstream/123456789/70572/4/Kutscheid-2-c5lxtqbie55a5_flat.pdf"/>
    <dc:creator>Kutscheid, Sophie</dc:creator>
    <bibo:uri rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/handle/123456789/70572"/>
  </rdf:Description>
</rdf:RDF>
Interner Vermerk
xmlui.Submission.submit.DescribeStep.inputForms.label.kops_note_fromSubmitter
Kontakt
URL der Originalveröffentl.
Prüfdatum der URL
Prüfungsdatum der Dissertation
April 18, 2024
Hochschulschriftenvermerk
Konstanz, Univ., Diss., 2024
Finanzierungsart
Kommentar zur Publikation
Allianzlizenz
Corresponding Authors der Uni Konstanz vorhanden
Internationale Co-Autor:innen
Universitätsbibliographie
Begutachtet
Diese Publikation teilen