Publikation: Counterfactual reasoning : developing a sense of "nearest possible world"
Dateien
Datum
Autor:innen
Herausgeber:innen
ISSN der Zeitschrift
Electronic ISSN
ISBN
Bibliografische Daten
Verlag
Schriftenreihe
Auflagebezeichnung
DOI (zitierfähiger Link)
Internationale Patentnummer
Angaben zur Forschungsförderung
Projekt
Open Access-Veröffentlichung
Sammlungen
Core Facility der Universität Konstanz
Titel in einer weiteren Sprache
Publikationstyp
Publikationsstatus
Erschienen in
Zusammenfassung
This study investigated at what point in development 3- to 6-year-old children begin to demonstrate counterfactual reasoning by controlling for fortuitously correct answers that result from basic conditional reasoning. Basic conditional reasoning occurs when one applies typical regularities (such as "If 'whenever' it doesn't rain the street is dry") to counterfactual questions (such as "If it had not rained, would the street be wet or dry?") without regard to actual events (e.g., if street cleaners had just been washing the street). In counterfactual reasoning, however, the conditional reasoning must be constrained by actual events (according to the "nearest possible world"). In situations when counterfactual reasoning and basic conditional reasoning would yield the same answers, even the youngest children gave mostly correct answers. However, tasks in which the 2 reasoning strategies resulted in different answers proved unusually difficult even for the older children.
Zusammenfassung in einer weiteren Sprache
Fachgebiet (DDC)
Schlagwörter
Konferenz
Rezension
Zitieren
ISO 690
RAFETSEDER, Eva, Renate CRISTI-VARGAS, Josef PERNER, 2010. Counterfactual reasoning : developing a sense of "nearest possible world". In: Child development. Wiley-Blackwell - SSH. 2010, 81(1), pp. 376-389. ISSN 0009-3920. eISSN 1467-8624. Available under: doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01401.xBibTex
@article{Rafetseder2010Count-52656, year={2010}, doi={10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01401.x}, title={Counterfactual reasoning : developing a sense of "nearest possible world"}, number={1}, volume={81}, issn={0009-3920}, journal={Child development}, pages={376--389}, author={Rafetseder, Eva and Cristi-Vargas, Renate and Perner, Josef} }
RDF
<rdf:RDF xmlns:dcterms="http://purl.org/dc/terms/" xmlns:dc="http://purl.org/dc/elements/1.1/" xmlns:rdf="http://www.w3.org/1999/02/22-rdf-syntax-ns#" xmlns:bibo="http://purl.org/ontology/bibo/" xmlns:dspace="http://digital-repositories.org/ontologies/dspace/0.1.0#" xmlns:foaf="http://xmlns.com/foaf/0.1/" xmlns:void="http://rdfs.org/ns/void#" xmlns:xsd="http://www.w3.org/2001/XMLSchema#" > <rdf:Description rdf:about="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/server/rdf/resource/123456789/52656"> <dcterms:abstract xml:lang="eng">This study investigated at what point in development 3- to 6-year-old children begin to demonstrate counterfactual reasoning by controlling for fortuitously correct answers that result from basic conditional reasoning. Basic conditional reasoning occurs when one applies typical regularities (such as "If 'whenever' it doesn't rain the street is dry") to counterfactual questions (such as "If it had not rained, would the street be wet or dry?") without regard to actual events (e.g., if street cleaners had just been washing the street). In counterfactual reasoning, however, the conditional reasoning must be constrained by actual events (according to the "nearest possible world"). In situations when counterfactual reasoning and basic conditional reasoning would yield the same answers, even the youngest children gave mostly correct answers. However, tasks in which the 2 reasoning strategies resulted in different answers proved unusually difficult even for the older children.</dcterms:abstract> <dc:contributor>Cristi-Vargas, Renate</dc:contributor> <dc:contributor>Rafetseder, Eva</dc:contributor> <foaf:homepage rdf:resource="http://localhost:8080/"/> <dc:creator>Perner, Josef</dc:creator> <dcterms:issued>2010</dcterms:issued> <dcterms:rights rdf:resource="https://rightsstatements.org/page/InC/1.0/"/> <dspace:isPartOfCollection rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/server/rdf/resource/123456789/43"/> <dcterms:isPartOf rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/server/rdf/resource/123456789/43"/> <dc:creator>Rafetseder, Eva</dc:creator> <dc:contributor>Perner, Josef</dc:contributor> <dc:language>eng</dc:language> <dc:creator>Cristi-Vargas, Renate</dc:creator> <dc:rights>terms-of-use</dc:rights> <void:sparqlEndpoint rdf:resource="http://localhost/fuseki/dspace/sparql"/> <dc:date rdf:datatype="http://www.w3.org/2001/XMLSchema#dateTime">2021-02-01T11:36:05Z</dc:date> <dcterms:title>Counterfactual reasoning : developing a sense of "nearest possible world"</dcterms:title> <bibo:uri rdf:resource="https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/handle/123456789/52656"/> <dcterms:available rdf:datatype="http://www.w3.org/2001/XMLSchema#dateTime">2021-02-01T11:36:05Z</dcterms:available> </rdf:Description> </rdf:RDF>